Good morning everyone.
Thank you so much Haizhou for your
very kind introduction.
And I also want to thank the organising committee
for giving me this chance to comment,
interact with colleagues from so many different fields, from so many different areas.
Speech
is a fundamental defining characteristic of our species.
Sometimes
our species is called
Homo sapiens,
but I'm not sure we deserve the word sapiens.
More likely,
some people said, we should be called
Homo loquens (talking man),
because
we're talking all the time, we're using speech all the time.
Because it's so fundamental, it's also
extremely complex.
I'm reminded of the cartoon that
Ray Liu showed yesterday
of an ocean of knowledge and
a few islands popping out.
Our job
is to build bridges
among those islands.
An island of electronic engineering,
island of linguistics,
computer science,
psychology,
neuroscience.
Each one
gives us an important window on speech.
And we need to connect them together.
The theme of this
conference
is the Diversity
of Spoken Language.
So this is the outlining
of my remarks today.
I will begin by asking, How is speech possible?
and I'll say something about
African origins,
the diversity that we see today in genes
and in words,
then I will proceed to
present three
case studies
of sound patterns in motion.
Sounds
hardly ever
behave as individuals.
They always
configure in patterns and move in patterns.
And I will
discuss consonants.
How they moved
with respect to Grimm's Law.
I will talk about vowels and
the Great Vowel Shift
in the history of English.
And I will talk about something that's not
found in all languages. Consonants and vowels are found in
all languages.
But in several thousand languages of the World you find tones.
And I will say something about how tones move.
And if there's time, I'd like to say something about speech and music.
Two cultural universals.
Two
cultural aspects that's present in every group
of human beings.
And I will close off.
I saw this cartoon few months ago and I thought maybe it's relevant.
Here you have one dolphin talking to another.
Although humans make sounds with their mouths,
occasionally look at each other,
there is no solid evidence that they actually communicate among themselves.
If somebody came up to me and looked at me and spoken Tamil.
I wouldn't understand him.
Or
Swahili,
or Caucasian,
or Abkhaz.
So in that sense our species is unique.
There is no other species
that have such great systematic diversity
among system of communication.
Diversity of language
is something unique to our species.
How did this
diversity come about?
Well, here's one story.
Lori Lamel
yesterday also
showed this beautiful painting by
Bruegel.
It is based on the story of The Book of Genesis.
God was annoyed
that people
had the hubris and
the arrogance
of building
a tower so high to reach the sky.
So God said, Go to, let us go down,
and there confound their language,
so that they may not understand
one another's speech.
Actually people all over the world
ethnographies told us,
have a very similar accounts
of creations and origins and so on.
My purpose this morning is to look at
diversity
more from
evolutionary perspective.
The key concept
behind diversity
is innovation.
In biological evolution
every generation of giraffes
has a certain degree of variation
in how long their neck is.
Every generation of wolves
have a certain variation
in how long their canine teeth are for fighting.
So nature selects,
there's variation.
As you innovate,
every generation has its variation.
And nature selects.
Similarly language.
Every infant,
in trying to reconstruct a language based on the
sounds, based on the language context in which it's born,
innovates.
And these innovations
mostly go by the wayside.
Many of them
are selected culturally rather than biologically
and persevere.
And as these innovations build up, accumulate more and more,
pretty soon
you won't be able to talk to each other anymore.
So, in biological evolution the innovations build up.
There's no longer mutual
reproductive
fertility.
You get a new species
in speech.
As these innovations become numerous,
build up over generation after generation,
you lose mutual intelligibility
and you have a new language.
So, the sounds as I said earlier
are not individuals.
They form natural patterns
and together
they
move in these patterns.
As these
innovations take place,
the social values attached to them.
Many of you may have enjoyed this great movie My Fair Lady.
Henry Higgins in the movie was actually based on the real English phonetician called Henry
Sweet.
But in the movie of course they took various liberties.
Here's Henry Higgins saying,
An Englishman's way of speaking
absolutely classifies him.
The moment he talks
he makes some other Englishman
despise him.
One common language I'm afraid we'll never get.
Oh, why can't the English learn to set a good example to people whose English
is painful to your ears?
The Scotch and the Irish
leave you close to tears.
Maybe that's why Scotch want independence.
Anyway according to Higgins,
There even are places where English completely disappears.
In America, they happen't used it for years!
Each one of us has to make a whole series of personal decisions when we
talk.
When I went to Hong Kong,
I noticed that in the textbook
to say you
in Cantonese
it's nay
with an n.
You find n in most of the Chinese dialects. In Mandarin it's nee
So I was told it's nay.
But as I
walked around the campus,
I noticed the young people don't say nay they all say lay.
The textbook says woo is gho
whether ??real/rear or nasal.
But the young people don't say gho, they say who.
So should I say
nay?
Closer to my native dialect
or should I try to be fashionable and say lay?
Lay is kinda not my age.
So, there are all these personal decisions
and these decisions build up
for group solidarity,
for group distinctions, and so on, and on and on
over the generations
new languages arise.
But let's take a look much for the back in time.
When did speech get it's first shot?
When did speech
start its trajectory?
I think a good case can be made for
over three million years ago,
okay.
In the region in Ethiopia
a bunch of physical anthropologists
were very lucky
and discovered
a relatively complete fossil of a
young woman,
middle teens,
called Lucy.
By studying Lucy
in great detail,
her hip bones
her ankle bones, her scull and so on,
they determined
that Lucy walked.
Lucy had erect posture,
essentially as we do.
So Lucy had made the transition to bipedal posture.
And
this is the reconstruction
of
Lucy.
This by itself, of course, is only part of the evidence.
Another part of the evidence which is also quite solid
is that they found, around the same time not very far from
this region called Afar,
footprints
covered by volcanic ash, so it's nicely preserved.
And here's the great
anthropologist by the name Mary Leakey
measuring in great detail
a whole string of footprints
and these footprints proved beyond doubt that over three million years ago
our ancestors were walking.
What are the implications of walking?
Well, in this excellent book written by Daniel Lieberman
called The Story of the Human Body
he contrasts
the
skeleton structure of the modern human
with that of the chimpanzee
and indeed there are many differences, many adaptations.
For us
perhaps the most important
is the restructuring of the entire upper body
and the head.
Rather than hanging forward
and help by muscle
the head's resting squarely
on the
cervical vertebrae,
or the spinal column.
It's
relatively
effortless for me to stand here,
not for a chimp.
I can lock my knees,
my skull is resting, its weight is on the spinal column.
There are many implications
of this new posture.
And immediate one of course, it freed our hands.
We became
tool makers in a serious sense.
Another very important adaptation
is that because of the restructuring of our heads
our larynx
has descended.
This is from a book
by Fitch, professor at Vienna.
And it shows the descent of the larynx
as well as the hyoid bone,
here's the thyroid cartilage and so on.
Even more clearly,
Daniel Lieberman's book,
makes it
very obvious why this is important for speech.
In the chimpanzee for instance whereas there's essentially just one resonance tube
to produce vocal sounds,
in our case
with the larynx
lowered
here's the larynx with the larynx lowered,
we now have two tubes.
A tube
that's the mouth
and another tube that's the throat.
And with the agile movement of the tongue
you can form much greater variety of phonetic distinctions.
And this of course is also extremely important,
but it's useless to have all that hardware
unless we have the proper
control mechanisms
and then control mechanisms
right in the brain.
Usually we see pictures of the
hemispheres of the brain,
but here's a nice picture
of the base of the brain.
If we lifted up the head
and looked at the brain from bottom-up,
we'll see a picture something like this.
Here's the temporal lobe, here's the frontal lobe.
The point that I wanna make
is that most of the cranial nerves,
twelve pairs coming out from the base brain,
most of them
are involved in speech.
That's how important the speech is
for
our life.
Henry Lenneberg,
some years ago,
published this
very nice diagram
showing the various nerves.
Here's the facial nerve,
which is cranial nerve pair number seven.
Here is (a) which is the trigeminal nerve
and perhaps the most
noticeable one
is this one that starts from around here in the base stand,
goes down all the way,
curls around the aorta of the heart
and goes back up
to control the larynx.
This is of course an effect of ascending up.
So five hairs of the twelve pairs of cranial nerves are involved in
controlling speech,
jaw movement, lip movement, larynx, tongue and so on.
So, probably
given both the
software in the head,
hardware in the throat and the mouth,
by about a hundred thousand years ago
our life
took a very important turn.
We left Africa.
Leaving Africa
is a hypothesis
that even Darwin mentioned
in the,
1871 book, Descent of Man.
But it's only within recent decade that the evidence become so strong.
The earliest evidence came in the 1980's
with mitochondrial DNA. Mitochondrial is only materially transmitted
and then came a whole spate of studies based on y-chromosome,
which is paternally transmitted
and now
they can do a huge amount of study
based on
single-nucleotide polymorphisms.
And it's largely
these genetic studies that leads to this picture
which was published in PNAS
a couple years ago
by a group of a geneticists at Stanford University.
So the scenario,
the best knowledge that we have for now,
is that humans
originated in Africa.
The earliest fossils take back to about hundred and fifty thousand years ago.
And
various genetic mutations took place
at around the same time. For instance
we have a particular version
of a gene called FOXP2,
okay.
Our version of the FOXP2 evolved probably
also around that time.
A whole bunch of things happened around that time
and
emigration started.
First to Asia,
back to Europe,
down to Oceania,
across the Bering Strait into the Americas.
And very quickly
all the way down to the, tip of South America, Tierra Del Fuego.
So here is an example of a genetic study,
done essentially by the same group at Stanford,
showing
how the
SNP, single-nucleotide
polymorphisms are related.
So here's the root of the tree
and in constructing these phylogenetic trees
often you have to take
outgroup.
Outgroup in this case is the chimpanzee.
Chimpanzee diverged from us about six million years ago.
So,
the chimpanzee provided a good
routing of the tree.
These are all Africans
and then step by step populate in the world.
Here's Asia,
here's the northeast,
here is
South America
and here are the
Pacific Islands.
What about language?
Obviously one people left.
They brought their languages with them.
And another
Stanford scholar professor Joseph Greenberg, was
actually my teacher,
classified
all of the seven thousand
or so languages of the world
into these major
superfamilies.
So, for instance, again just as with people,
languages have the greatest diversity in Africa.
A very successful
family of languages
is called Eurasiatic.
Europe .. Asia. Eurasiatic.
And within this family
there's one sub-family that's
extremely
dominant. That's called Indo-European.
Indo-European because at one end you have India
at the other end
you have Europe extending all the way to Iceland.
So English
is Eurasiatic,
because English
is West Germanic. Germanic .. Indo-European ..
Eurasiatic.
Going up the family tree.
Another language here .. Tamil.
Tamil is not related to any of the Eurasiatic languages.
Tamil is a member of Dravidian family.
Earliest forms of Tamil actually
were found
much more
northwestern.
Where it's not spoken.
It was founded in the Indus Valley.
With the coming of the Indo-Europeans
they were push further and further south, so that
Tamil is now in this small region here.
Malay as we saw in the last slide
is an Oceanian language
that
has a relatively recent history,
but what about Chinese?
Chinese belongs to a family called Sino-Tibetan.
Chinese is related to Tibetan.
Well, recognized
over a hundred years ago.
And
more recently
people like Greenberg say Chinese
is Dene-Caucasian.
Chinese is here in red,
but there are spots of red
all over the world.
According to several very eminent linguists, Greenberg's one of them,
Sergei Starostin in Moscow is another one, okay,
say all these languages are related.
Chinese is related
not only to Tibetan,
but also to the Yeniseian languages.
But not only to the Yeniseian languages,
but all the way across Bering Strait
the Athabaskan languages.
Including for instance
Navajo.
So,
together with genes, archaeology and so on
we are getting a better notion
of what we come from
and speech plays a very important role here.
Given all these languages,
how do we classify them?
Well, one way to classify is very straightforward, you know, where is it spoken?
Geographical.
This gives us a geographical distribution.
Another way is
typological.
What kind of structure does it have?
Does it have a gender system?
Is indirect object before the direct object?
Or is the other way?
Does it have tongues?
These are typological features.
So, this is a second way of classifying languages.
And the third way of classifying languages,
what is it related to?
Is Chinese related to Tibetan?
Is Tamil related to Telugu?
Which it is.
So on.
Sometimes
genetic or
historical relationship
and typological relationship
classified differently.
English for instance, as I said a few minutes ago,
is the Germanic language.
But it doesn't look anything like German.
English does not have chen (suffix),
a gender system.
German has three genders: masculine, feminine, neuter.
In English the primary
order for declarative sentences .. you put the verb in the middle,
I saw John.
In most of other Germanic languages, including German,
in declarative sentences
the verb is at the end.
All these structural differences.
So, when we want to use corpora of languages
to
use cross-linguistically
it's primarily the typological
classification that's important.
Not the genetic relation that's important.
Well, let's ask the question,
How did all this
diversity arise?,
Where did all this diversity come from?
For instance
here's a distribution by
area. This is the geographical distribution.
This was shown by an earlier speaker too.
According to the Ethnologue which is
essentially
missionary organisation where the team of thousands of linguists
travelling far-flung places of the world.
With one of their missions to translate The Bible
and in this process
they live there, they know the region well, they can report on the language and
according to the
Ethnologue there are seven thousand or so languages.
We shouldn't take these numbers very seriously. They are very good
rough guide.
For instance
when I went to Hong Kong, I couldn't understand the
word of the Chinese that they spoke there .. Cantonese.
Cantonese and Mandarin are supposed to be Chinese.
One language.
If this is so, then the
dozens of dialects in China only count once, even though they're mutually unintelligible.
On the other hand one year I was teaching in Stockholm.
Bought myself a little Volvo,
drove across from Stockholm to Oslo.
Swedish .. Norwegian.
No problem at all communicating.
Yet there we have two languages, okay.
So these numbers
just can't be taken too seriously,
but it's good to have kind of as a
guideline.
Was it
also this complicated at the beginning when we first heard language?
What does this diversity come from?
Well, linguists have two ways of talking about the origin.
Either
monogenesis,
all of the languages in the world
came from a single source
or at the beginning
there were various tribes in different regions
of the world maybe,
of Africa maybe,
and each of them came upon the idea of inventing language.
And some years back
I collaborated with the
mathematician, friend of mine at Berkeley, David Freedman.
And we did some statistical modeling
and
our thought
that is
much more likely,
given you have
a large number of sites,
so much more likely
the language arose
polygenetically.
This means, if this is right,
that some diversity was there
right at the beginning.
And this is not completely unreasonable because
you think about the invention of fire
that's had terrific consequences.
That was polygenetically invented.
Many people invented them independently.
You think about pottery.
Partly another major
prehistoric invention.
Independent.
More polygenesis.
You think about written language.
Many cultures independently invented written language.
So, spoken language, you know, not in the nuanced
powerful sense of spoken language now.
Spoken language
hundred thousand years ago
could have been invented polygenetically.
One person who took the monogenetic
approach
was Quentin Atkinson
who published this paper in Science few years back.
He
use the
corpus
world atlas of language structures which is maintained in Leipzig in Germany.
And looked at five hundred and four languages
in terms of their phoneme inventories.
How many consonants, how many vowels and so on.
And you can tell from the title of his paper
Phonemic Diversity Supports
a Serial Founder Effect Model
of Language Expansion.
In another words
he's trying to
construct the picture
kind of like this that we saw earlier for genes.
A serial founding effect.
He wants to do it
for languages and he does it
with this diagram
that you see on the
left side of the slide
from where you seti.
Without going into great detail of Atkinson's methodology,
I should mention that
his proposal was met with several very severe
criticisms
from three groups of scholars.
Some in Europe, some in America and so on.
And they are listed on the slide.
For instance
Cysouw
Dediu and Moran,
in the later issue of Science,
contested
both their databases
and their method
and came up with a very different picture
based on phoneme inventories.
Atkinson's
hypothesis would put
origin of language here, but actually there are many other sites.
In order to give us a clearer idea of how sound patterns actually move
I will now move to
three case studies.
Atkinson took the seven thousand languages of
global perspective, all the languages of the world.
I think perhaps we're not quite ready
to be that global.
It might be safer to stick with one family on which there is a very
detailed meticulous scholarship
that has been going on
since
the
eighteen century, latter part of the eighteenth century.
So, in Science 2004
we have a
picture of the Indo-European languages.
I'm not sure
whether you can catch everything
but there is not an English.
Traces to
Middle English of Chaucer,
Old English of King Alfred
and all the way up to Indo-European.
Indo-European also includes
Tocharian.
Which is an language that's become extinct.
You find remnants of it in western in China, in Xinjiang.
And you have
the
Indo-Iranian branch. Within the Indo-Iranian branch
you see Kurdish,
which is very much in the news these days. They're the ones fighting in Iraq.
So, Kurdish is actually an Iranian language belonging to the Proto-Indo-Iranian family.
This gives you an idea of the membership
doesn't tell us very much about the time scale.
In Nature in 2003
Russell Grey
and Atkinson
did an extensive
statistical analysis based on vocabulary.
They took almost two thousand five hundred words
from eighty seven languages,
all of from Indo-European and constructed this tree.
Along this tree
you see estimated time of death.
So, according to these people published in Nature in
2003
Indo-European
is about nine thousand years old.
As it gradually split
into
more language families
based on innovations,
okay.
A few thousand years ago
it
led to Germanic languages.
Eventually to English.
Those are
the,
the slide is kind of hard to see,
so I will take this section here
and blow it up
and we have this.
So, here we have English,
here we have German,
and we have the Romance languages,
Italian, Spanish,
and a separation here seems to be about
five thousand years ago.
What I'd like to
discuss now
is what other innovations
that led to just
this one little group.
How did
Indo-European
become Germanic
in a part of the world?
And this is how linguists operate, okay.
Here's some data.
Colin Renfrew is very esteemed archaeologist at Cambridge University
and he gave as a bunch of words.
The integers one to ten
in a bunch of languages.
English one, two, three, four, up to ten.
Japanese
hitotsu, futatsu, mittsu, yottsu.
Doesn't look anything like the rest of them.
So, Japanese
it's not an Indo-European language.
But the rest.
English
two
corresponds to Gothic.
Gothic is an extinct language but we know it because there's lot of textual material
preserved.
Gothic has twai.
Two ..
twai.
Why isn't the w pronounced in two
but is still written?
In Latin is duo.
In Greek
is duo,
in Sanskrit
it's dva.
In Russian is also dva.
Obviously there's similarity in there.
But is there something systematic that we can extract?
Yes, the systematic aspect is that
Germanic,
English and Gothic
T
corresponds
to other Indo-European languages'
D.
Let's take the next one.
Three
th is just a abbreviation
referring to the sound that Anne Cutler was talking about interdental fricative θ.
Three that's a interdental fricative.
So, here's a fricative, here's a fricative in Germanic
and these fricatives correspond to T here, T here,
T here,
systematically
as a pattern.
Without going through many more
this is the correspondence that we can extract
and
here's the great man
who formulated the law
Jacob Grimm.
Older brother of Wilhelm Grimm.
The brothers of the Grimm's Fairy Tales, okay. Jacob was also a great linguist.
In 1822
he published what's called the Grimm's Law.
He said
these
words
in Indo-European
pronounced with a
B(h)
D(h), G(h) type of sound,
became in Germanic
P,
D and G.
Sounds which were B, D, G in Indo-European
became P, T, K
in Germanic.
And sounds which where P, T, K in Germanic,
in Indo-European, sorry. Became F, θ, H
in Germanic.
Very regular.
We are able to look at some of these in comparison because
after the Norman conquest, about a thousand years ago,
English powered a lot of romance words.
French words, Latin words.
So we actually have
Latin and Romance words sitting side by side with the native Germanic words.
So take a word like
ped,
the pedal
on a bicycle.
It's foot.
So the P corresponds to the F.
P corresponds to the F.
The pad, the D
corresponds to the T.
D corresponds to the T.
So, the point that I wanna make with this slide,
I better move faster,
is that
it's a whole natural class that's displaced.
It's displaced not only as total patterns
but it's usually displaced
by single features.
As you go from here to here
you're going from
voice task ??
to voice ??.
As you go from here to here
you're going from voiced to unvoiced.
As you go from here to here you're going from stop to fricative and so
on.
It's
beautifully symmetric.
So, let's get closer to home.
Here's the sound change that took place.
It sided shortly after Chaucer's death
and it kept on going after Shakespeare.
It's called the Great Vowel Shift.
It's been studied in ??Greatstepft by Danish linguistic called
Otto Jespersen.
And some years back I also
followed up that study.
Why is it that we say
sanity,
but sane.
Why is it that in French
China's called Chine?
Chine versus China.
Christmas
versus Christ.
Why are there all these alternations?
It's because of the Great Vowel Shift.
And to hear one example.
The original vowel quality was actually a.
It used to be sanity.
But if you take away the suffix,
which protects this vowel,
the vowel itself raises.
So, it became sane.
So, we still say sanity
but we no longer say, he's insane.
We have to say insane.
As wind shift
we can say serenity
but we cannot say, this man is very serene.
We have to say serene
A has become E, E has become I. I can't go any higher.
So,
Christmas, the I,
has become Christ.
Chine
in French has become China in English, because of this vowel shift.
Again it's a systematic shift in patterns.
Okay, let's
spend a few minutes on
perhaps less familiar territory.
You build words not only with consonants and vowels
but also with tones.
People think of tones they think of Chinese
but there are thousands of tone languages in the world.
Here's a tone language called Trique,
in Mexico.
And in Trique
there are five levels of tones.
So, we have,
this is,
no,
gu du we ku.
Gu du we jo.
And so on.
Each with a very different meaning.
So, ku is bones.
Jo, ka, ?a, za.
Five different level tones.
With five different words.
But is it the case that
once you have a tone language always a tone language? No.
The paradigm tone language Chinese. People thought, Well, Chinese is a tone language.
Was it always a tone language? No.
Chinese became a tone language probably about
two thousand
twenty five hundred years ago.
And this is the result of
the loss of consonant clusters.
In Enghlish you have a lot of consonant clusters like
speech, play, spring, these all begin with consonant clusters.
But if you look at the Chinese dialects now, any dialect you want look at,
no consonant clusters.
So, how do we know that there was consonant clusters?
Chinese is not written alphabetically. How can you tell what it sounded like?
Well, it's true Chinese is not written alphabetically but a large part is written
phonetically.
It's not in terms of consonants and vowels. It's in terms of syllables.
So, for instance
here's,
I call these things sinograms
for Chinese characters. Here's a sinogram
that serves
as the phonetic
of this more complex sinogram.
Here's a sinogram
that serves as a phonetic.
Sinogram that serves as a phonetic and so on.
But then you ask me,
How is that phonetic?
They're different.
These are
spelled with a letter G but actually there's K sounds there.
Unvoiced un-aspirated velar stops,
okay.
These are all L.
Well, that's one of the
evidence
that they were consonant clusters.
If you take these words,
compare them with Tibetan.
Many languages in Southeast Asia
they also have constant clusters.
So, because these consonant clusters were lost,
tones arose.
Otherwise you'd just have too much ??.
And they arose in different ways.
So, here's Beijing,
Xiang, Xi'an, ??Hankou\Hong Kong.
and so on.
Each one
with the different number of tones.
In Mandarin they're four.
In Cantonese there are none.
So, this illustrates the four tones of
Mandarin.
This illustrates the nine tones of Hong Kong Cantonese.
Notice in Mandarin
because of the merger
??
??
??
have all become homophones.
Exactly the same pronunciation.
But in Cantonese
one is mid-level
??
??
??
three different tones
they kept.
And if you
did F0 analysis to extract the pitch.
This is actually.
my own voice
extracted from a PDP computer. Many of you may not even remember what PDP computers
are.
Back in 1973.
So, it is my voice saying
??
??
And here are the nine tones
from Cantonese.
You could plot these
on a
two-dimensional graph
using slope
and normalized height.
And you can see that the four tones in Mandarin
??
are quite evenly distributed.
On the other hand in Cantonese
it's kind of a mess.
There's a lot of overlapping
and this happens a lot whenever
a metropolitan centre
suddenly takes in a lot of influx of people
speak different dialects, different languages. The language will change faster.
So, as a result
in Cantonese tone two
and tone five
are merging.
Many of the words
are no longer distinct.
Tone three and tone six
are merging.
Many of them are no longer distinct.
I see I've ran out of time, Haizhou.
The last point on this tone part,
I'll just mentioned very quickly,
is that we saw a little bit earlier the vowel shift changed the vowels chasing
each other around, okay.
Low become middle, middle becomes high, high becomes
??deflunguised
Well, you can't say things in tones.
In Taiwanese
okay, here's
database on the Ministry of Education in Taiwan,
if you take each tones
and notice how they change.
They actually change in circle.
And detailed study of this
is available
in the literature.
The last point that
I wanted to discuss, but I can discuss with you individually,
is that many people think that language actually arose
from music.
The proto-language of the humankind
was actually singing.
Darwin himself
made many remarks to this
point.
So, in English, archaeologist Steven Mithen,
wrote the Singing Neanderthals
Origins of Music, Language, Mind, and Body.
And the linguist Fitch at University of Vienna
in his book Evolution of Language,
chapter fourteen
is all about
proto-language.
And here's a slide prepared by a graduate student at The Chinese University
showing
the
grammatical operators,
like drawing this type of trees,
applies very well
to music.
So, here's one of Bach's Menuets that
has
syntactic structures very much like
real sentences.
And this last slide shows that
actually
if you look at it just from the point of view region of interest with
fMRI
they're actually
distinct regions from music and language.
All this is very interesting, not too surprising
but we really need to go further
and look at the fiber tracks
not just the regions.
But how these regions are connected.
Okay, I have two more slides.
So, the roots of language then,
as a summary,
reach back over three million years,
when our remote ancestors transitioned
to bipedal posture,
restructuring the hands,
the vocal tract,
and the brain.
Speech
with its building blocks of syllables,
vowels,
consonants,
is a powerful vehicle for language,
emerged over a hundred thousand years ago.
Language and music are both universal to our species
and share evolutionary roots.
They have similar functions of communication,
similar principles of organisation.
Diversity in language is the cumulative product
of culturally selected innovations
made by numerous generations of speakers.
Spoken language has spawned
various auxiliary forms,
such as written language,
signed language, there is electronic media providing
additional windows for studying how we communicate.
And ever more powerful
technology of brain imaging and
computer analysis for spoken language and music
is already shedding much light
on our mind
and promises
to reveal
much more.
Thank you.
As much as we are a bit overtime I still welcome very short comments
after a mind-boggling talk that perhaps
bring back
speech and language
all the way to the roots.
If not
on behalf of Interspeech
we have a token of appreciation
that we would like to ask
the conference chair Haizhou Li to present to professor
William Wong.